How positive affect modulates cognitive control: reduced perseveration at the cost of increased distractibility. G Dreisbach, T Goschke Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 30 (2), 343, 2004 | 1120 | 2004 |
Conflicts as aversive signals G Dreisbach, R Fischer Brain and cognition 78 (2), 94-98, 2012 | 365 | 2012 |
How positive affect modulates cognitive control: The costs and benefits of reduced maintenance capability G Dreisbach Brain and cognition 60 (1), 11-19, 2006 | 333 | 2006 |
Conflicts as aversive signals: Motivation for control adaptation in the service of affect regulation G Dreisbach, R Fischer Motivation and cognitive control, 188-210, 2015 | 225 | 2015 |
Dopamine and cognitive control: the influence of spontaneous eyeblink rate and dopamine gene polymorphisms on perseveration and distractibility. G Dreisbach, J Müller, T Goschke, A Strobel, K Schulze, KP Lesch, ... Behavioral neuroscience 119 (2), 483, 2005 | 223 | 2005 |
Preparatory processes in the task-switching paradigm: evidence from the use of probability cues. G Dreisbach, H Haider, RH Kluwe Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 28 (3), 468, 2002 | 208 | 2002 |
On how to be flexible (or not): Modulation of the stability-flexibility balance G Dreisbach, K Fröber Current Directions in Psychological Science 28 (1), 3-9, 2019 | 199 | 2019 |
Conflicts as aversive signals: Conflict priming increases negative judgments for neutral stimuli J Fritz, G Dreisbach Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience 13, 311-317, 2013 | 192 | 2013 |
The differential influences of positive affect, random reward, and performance-contingent reward on cognitive control K Fröber, G Dreisbach Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience 14, 530-547, 2014 | 171 | 2014 |
Mechanisms of cognitive control: The functional role of task rules G Dreisbach Current Directions in Psychological Science 21 (4), 227-231, 2012 | 160 | 2012 |
How task representations guide attention: further evidence for the shielding function of task sets. G Dreisbach, H Haider Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 35 (2), 477, 2009 | 158 | 2009 |
Conflict-triggered goal shielding: Response conflicts attenuate background monitoring for prospective memory cues T Goschke, G Dreisbach Psychological Science 19 (1), 25-32, 2008 | 143 | 2008 |
The role of affect and reward in the conflict-triggered adjustment of cognitive control G Dreisbach, R Fischer Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 6, 342, 2012 | 138 | 2012 |
That’s what task sets are for: shielding against irrelevant information G Dreisbach, H Haider Psychological research 72, 355-361, 2008 | 136 | 2008 |
Preparatory adjustment of cognitive control in the task switching paradigm G Dreisbach, H Haider Psychonomic bulletin & review 13, 334-338, 2006 | 136 | 2006 |
Genetic variation of serotonin function and cognitive control A Strobel, G Dreisbach, J Müller, T Goschke, B Brocke, KP Lesch Journal of cognitive neuroscience 19 (12), 1923-1931, 2007 | 118 | 2007 |
Dopamine and cognitive control: the prospect of monetary gains influences the balance between flexibility and stability in a set‐shifting paradigm J Müller, G Dreisbach, T Goschke, T Hensch, KP Lesch, B Brocke European Journal of Neuroscience 26 (12), 3661-3668, 2007 | 117 | 2007 |
Keep flexible–keep switching! The influence of forced task switching on voluntary task switching K Fröber, G Dreisbach Cognition 162, 48-53, 2017 | 116 | 2017 |
The role of task rules and stimulus–response mappings in the task switching paradigm G Dreisbach, T Goschke, H Haider Psychological Research 71 (4), 383-392, 2007 | 113 | 2007 |
Backward inhibition as a means of sequential task-set control: evidence for reduction of task competition. M Hübner, G Dreisbach, H Haider, RH Kluwe Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 29 (2), 289, 2003 | 107 | 2003 |