A review of control processes and their locus in language switching M Declerck, AM Philipp Psychonomic bulletin & review 22, 1630-1645, 2015 | 305 | 2015 |
Digits vs. pictures: The influence of stimulus type on language switching M Declerck, I Koch, AM Philipp Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 15 (4), 896-904, 2012 | 135 | 2012 |
The minimum requirements of language control: evidence from sequential predictability effects in language switching. M Declerck, I Koch, AM Philipp Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 41 (2), 377, 2015 | 104 | 2015 |
Is language control just a form of executive control? Evidence for overlapping processes in language switching and task switching M Declerck, J Grainger, I Koch, AM Philipp Journal of Memory and Language 95, 138-145, 2017 | 100 | 2017 |
What about proactive language control? M Declerck Psychonomic bulletin & review 27 (1), 24-35, 2020 | 94 | 2020 |
Bilingual control: sequential memory in language switching. M Declerck, AM Philipp, I Koch Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 39 (6), 1793, 2013 | 76 | 2013 |
The effect of dual task demands and proficiency on second language speech production M Declerck, J Kormos Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 15 (4), 782-796, 2012 | 75 | 2012 |
Highly proficient bilinguals implement inhibition: Evidence from n-2 language repetition costs. M Declerck, AM Thoma, I Koch, AM Philipp Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 41 (6), 1911, 2015 | 72 | 2015 |
Can monolinguals be like bilinguals? Evidence from dialect switching NW Kirk, V Kempe, KC Scott-Brown, A Philipp, M Declerck Cognition 170, 164-178, 2018 | 71 | 2018 |
What absent switch costs and mixing costs during bilingual language comprehension can tell us about language control. M Declerck, I Koch, JA Duñabeitia, J Grainger, DN Stephan Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 45 (6), 771, 2019 | 70 | 2019 |
Assessing the not‐invented‐here syndrome: Development and validation of implicit and explicit measurements D Antons, M Declerck, K Diener, I Koch, FT Piller Journal of Organizational Behavior 38 (8), 1227-1245, 2017 | 70 | 2017 |
The concept of inhibition in bilingual control. M Declerck, I Koch Psychological Review 130 (4), 953, 2023 | 68 | 2023 |
Dissociating language-switch costs from cue-switch costs in bilingual language switching KW Heikoop, M Declerck, SA Los, I Koch Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 19 (5), 921-927, 2016 | 67 | 2016 |
Parallel semantic processing in reading revisited: Effects of translation equivalents in bilingual readers J Snell, M Declerck, J Grainger Language, Cognition and Neuroscience 33 (5), 563-574, 2018 | 61 | 2018 |
Which bilinguals reverse language dominance and why? M Declerck, D Kleinman, TH Gollan Cognition 204, 104384, 2020 | 60 | 2020 |
A sentence to remember: Instructed language switching in sentence production M Declerck, AM Philipp Cognition 137, 166-173, 2015 | 60 | 2015 |
Assessing the evidence for asymmetrical switch costs and reversed language dominance effects–a meta-analysis M Gade, M Declerck, AM Philipp, A Rey-Mermet, I Koch Journal of Cognition 4 (1), 2021 | 51 | 2021 |
Bilingual language interference initiates error detection: Evidence from language intrusions M Declerck, K Lemhöfer, J Grainger Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 20 (5), 1010-1016, 2017 | 50 | 2017 |
Inducing asymmetrical switch costs in bilingual language comprehension by language practice M Declerck, J Grainger Acta Psychologica 178, 100-106, 2017 | 48 | 2017 |
Unified syntax in the bilingual mind M Declerck, Y Wen, J Snell, G Meade, J Grainger Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 27 (1), 149-154, 2020 | 44 | 2020 |